Statistics for Corpus Linguists
  • Overview
  • Fundamentals
    • 1.1 Basics
    • 1.2 Research questions
    • 1.3 Linguistic variables
    • 1.4 Set theory and mathematical notation
  • Introduction to R
    • 2.1 First steps
    • 2.2 Exploring R Studio
    • 2.3 Vectors
    • 2.4 Data frames
    • 2.5 Libraries
    • 2.6 Importing/Exporting
  • NLP
    • 3.1 Concordancing
    • 3.2 Regular expressions
    • 3.3 Data annotation
  • Statistics
    • 4.1 Data, variables, samples
    • 4.2 Probability theory
    • 4.3 Descriptive statistics
    • 4.4 Hypothesis testing
    • 4.5 Binomial test
    • 4.6 Chi-squared test
    • 4.7 t-test
  • Models
    • 6.1 Linear regression
    • 6.2 Logistic regression
    • 6.3 Mixed-effects regression
    • 6.4 Poisson regression
    • 6.5 Ordinal regression
  • Machine Learning
    • 7.1 Tree-based methods
    • 7.2 Gradient boosting
    • 7.3 PCA
    • 7.4 EFA
    • 7.5 Clustering
  1. 6. Statistical Modelling
  2. 6.2 Logistic regression
  • 6. Statistical Modelling
    • 6.1 Linear regression
    • 6.2 Logistic regression
    • 6.3 Mixed-effects regression
    • 6.4 Poisson regression
    • 6.5 Ordinal regression

On this page

  • Recommended reading
  • Preparation
    • Descriptive overview
  • Logistic regression
    • The simple logistic model
    • Multiple logistic regression
    • Odds ratios
  • Maximum Likelihood Estimation
  • Workflow in R
    • Research question and hypotheses
    • Convert to factors and specify reference levels
    • Model fitting
    • Further model diagnostics
    • Confidence intervals and odds ratios
    • Visualisation
    • Interpret the model
  1. 6. Statistical Modelling
  2. 6.2 Logistic regression

6.2 Logistic Regression

Author
Affiliation

Vladimir Buskin

Catholic University of Eichstätt-Ingolstadt

Abstract
Modelling categorical (binary) response variables.

Recommended reading

For linguists:

Levshina (2015: Chapter 12)

Winter (2020: Chapter 12)

Full theoretical treatment:

James et al. (2021: Chapter 4)

Hosmer & Lemeshow (2008)

Preparation

Consider the data from Buskin’s (2025)1 corpus-study on subject pronoun realisation:

  • 1 The input data can be downloaded from this OSF repository: https://osf.io/qgnms.

  • # Load libraries
    library(tidyverse)
    library(broom)
    library(DescTools)
    library(lmtest)
    
    # Load data
    data_pro <- read.csv("INPUT_pronouns.csv", sep = ",", header = TRUE)
    
    # Inspect data
    str(data_pro)
    head(data_pro)
    • Target variable:

      • Reference (‘overt’, ‘null’)
    • Explanatory variables:

      • Person (‘1.p.’, ‘2.p’, ‘3.p’ as well as the dummy pronouns ‘it’ and ‘there’)

      • Register (the text category in the International Corpus of English; ‘S1A’ are informal conversations, whereas ‘S1B’ comprises formal class lessons)

      • Variety (British English ‘GB’, Singapore English ‘SING’ and Hong Kong English ‘HK’), and

      • Referentiality (‘referential’ with an identifiable referent or ‘non-referential’ with no/generic reference)

    head(data_pro)
      Reference Person Register Variety Referentiality
    1     overt      3      S1A      GB    referential
    2     overt      3      S1A      GB    referential
    3     overt      3      S1A      GB    referential
    4     overt      3      S1A      GB    referential
    5     overt      3      S1A      GB    referential
    6     overt      3      S1A      GB    referential
    table(data_pro$Reference)
    
     null overt 
      174  4664 

    Descriptive overview

    Show the code
    data_pro_stats1 <- data_pro %>% 
      group_by(Variety) %>% 
      count(Reference) %>% 
      mutate(pct = n/sum(n) * 100)
    Variety Reference n pct
    GB null 29 1.817043
    GB overt 1567 98.182957
    HK null 75 4.507212
    HK overt 1589 95.492789
    SING null 70 4.435995
    SING overt 1508 95.564005

    Show the code
    data_pro_stats2 <- data_pro %>% 
      group_by(Variety, Register) %>% 
      count(Reference) %>% 
      mutate(pct = n/sum(n) * 100)

    Logistic regression

    In contrast to linear regression, logistic regression models a qualitative response variable \(Y\) with two outcomes2. In the present study, \(Y\) is pronominal Reference and has the outcomes Reference = null and Reference = overt, which represent null and overt subjects, respectively. Dichotomous variables of this kind are also often coded as yes/no or 1/0.

  • 2 Logistic regression can also be used for \(\geq 3\) classes by breaking down the response variable into a series of dichotomous variables. This is also known as multinomial logistic regression or softmax regression.

  • Another difference from linear regression is the output of the model:

    • In linear regression, we obtain a predicted value for the continuous response variable we’re interested in. For instance, if we’re modelling reaction times, the model will return an estimated mean reaction time (given the predictors).

    • In logistic regression, the model will return a probability. In variational linguistics, this may correspond to the probability that a speaker will use one syntactic variant versus the other.

    A core component of logistic regression is the logistic function. The rationale for using it is that the output of the function will always lie between \(0\) and \(1\), and it will always denote a probability.

    The simple logistic model

    Assuming a binary response variable \(Y\) with the values 1 and 0 and a single predictor \(X\), the conditional probability \(P(Y = 1 \mid X)\) is then equivalent to the inverse logit in Equation 1.

    \[ P(Y = 1 \mid X) = \frac{e^{\beta_0 + \beta_1X}}{1 + e^{\beta_0 + \beta_1X}}. \tag{1}\]

    With some manipulation it can be shaped into a form that is definitely more familiar:

    \[ \log\left(\frac{P(Y = 1 \mid X)}{1 - P(Y = 1 \mid X)}\right) = \beta_0 + \beta_1X. \tag{2}\]

    The logistic model has several characteristic components. The fraction \(\frac{P(Y = 1 \mid X)}{1-P(Y = 1 \mid X)}\) represents the odds, which stand for to the probability of one outcome (e.g., Reference = null) compared to the other (e.g., Reference = overt). Their logarithmic transformation are the log odds (or logits) of one outcome versus the other.

    Understanding log odds

    When interpreting the output of a logistic model, note that

    • positive log odds indicate an increase in \(\frac{P(Y = 1 \mid X)}{1-P(Y = 1 \mid X)}\), whereas

    • negative log odds indicate a decrease in \(\frac{P(Y = 1 \mid X)}{1-P(Y = 1 \mid X)}\).

    In more concrete terms: If we are interested in the probability that the form of pronominal reference is null (our \(Y\)) while taking into account the extra-linguistic context (Register; our \(X\)), the model would then have the general form in Equation 3.

    \[ \log\left(\frac{P(\text{Reference} = \text{null} \mid \text{Register})}{1- P(\text{Reference} = \text{null} \mid \text{Register})}\right) = \beta_0 + \beta_1\text{Register} \tag{3}\]

    Multiple logistic regression

    If more than one predictor is included, the above equations can be expanded so as to take into account \(p\) slopes for \(p\) independent variables \(X_1, X_2, ..., X_p\).

    \[ P(Y = 1 \mid X_1, ..., X_p) = \frac{e^{\beta_0 + \beta_1X_1 + ... + \beta_pX_p}}{1 + e^{\beta_0 + \beta_1X_1 + ... + \beta_pX_p}}. \tag{4}\]

    Consequently, the log odds correspond to the sum of linear predictors \(\beta_1X_1 + \beta_2X_2 + ...+ \beta_pX_p\) (cf. Equation 5).

    \[ \log\left(\frac{P(Y = 1 \mid X_1, ..., X_p)}{1 - P(Y = 1 \mid X_1, ..., X_p)}\right) = \beta_0 + \sum_{i=1}^{p} \beta_i X_i \tag{5}\]

    Odds ratios

    To assess the strength of an effect, it is instructive to examine the odds ratios that correspond to the model coefficients. Odds ratios (OR) are defined as

    \[ OR(X_1) = e^{\beta_1}. \]

    Understanding odds ratios

    Essentially, the OR describes the ratio between two odds with respect to another independent variable. This is illustrated for Reference given Register below:

    \[ \text{OR}(\text{Reference} \mid \text{Register}) = \frac{\frac{P(\text{Reference} = \text{null} \mid \text{Register} = \text{S1A})}{P(\text{Reference} = \text{overt} \mid \text{Register} = \text{S1A})}}{\frac{P(\text{Reference} = \text{null} \mid \text{Register} = \text{S1B})}{P(\text{Reference} = \text{overt} \mid \text{Register} = \text{S1B})}} \]

    Read as: ‘The ratio between the probability of a null vs. overt subject in S1A and the probability of a null vs. overt object in S1B’.

    Maximum Likelihood Estimation

    In contrast to continuous data, the estimation of parameters for discrete response variables is less straightforward in that there is no unique solution. Rather than finding a regression line that minimises the distance to all data points, the default approach of logistic models is to find the parameter values that are most likely, given the data. Hence this procedure is also known as Maximum Likelihood Estimation (MLE).

    The model first makes an assumption about the probability distribution of the data. For categorical data, the binomial distribution is a common choice. Equation 6 indicates the corresponding probability mass function, which describes the probability \(\pi\) of observing \(y\) successes in \(k\) independent Bernoulli trials. In other words, if we tossed a coin \(n = 10\) times and observed \(y = 5\) heads (i.e., 5 successes), what is the probability \(\pi\) of a success?

    \[ f(y; k; \pi) = \binom{k}{y} \pi^y (1 - \pi)^{n-y} \tag{6}\]

    Now, let \(\beta\) denote some parameter of interest (e.g., the slope coefficient of a logistic regression model). Given some observed data, the likelihood of this parameter can be described in terms of the likelihood function \(L(\beta)\) in Equation 7. It assumes \(n\) binomial probability mass functions with trials \(k = 1\) and computes their product. Since the binomial coefficient \(\binom{n}{y}\) is a constant term, it is typically dropped. In essence, we’re multiplying successes \(\pi^{y_i}_i\) with failures \((1 - \pi_i)^{1-y_i}\) for each data point.

    \[ L(\beta) = \prod_{i=1}^n \pi^{y_i}_i (1 - \pi_i)^{1-y_i} \tag{7}\]

    Within the context of our pronoun data, this can be understood more intuitively as the product of the probability that a subject is a null pronoun with the probability that it is not:

    \[ L(\beta) = \prod_{i=1}^n \pi^{\text{null}}_i (1 - \pi_i)^{1-\text{null}} \tag{8}\]

    Conventionally, this expression is log-transformed in order to convert the product into a sum because, for one, sums are easier to handle computationally. The log-likelihood function \(\ell(\beta)\) in Equation 9 forms the basis for a variety of goodness-of-fit measures used to evaluate logistic regression models.

    \[ \ell(\beta) = \sum_{i=1}^n y_i \log(\pi_i) + (1 - y_i) \log(1 - \pi_i) \tag{9}\]

    And in more concrete terms:

    \[ \ell(\beta) = \sum_{i=1}^n \text{null}_i \log(\pi_i) + (1 - \text{null}_i) \log(1 - \pi_i) \tag{10}\]

    In a next step, all \(\pi_i\)s would be substituted by their respectively probabilities from the right half of Equation 4.

    The goal is to find the value that maximises \(\ell(\beta)\), i.e., the maximum likelihood estimator \(\hat{\beta}\). Approximate solutions can be attained via iterative optimisation techniques (e.g., Newton-Ralphson or Gradient Descent). Sometimes the algorithm may fail to find an optimal solution, which R may report as a model convergence error. For further technical details, see Wood (2006: 63-66) or Agresti & Kateri (2022: 291-294).

    Workflow in R

    Research question and hypotheses

    How do the intra- and extra-linguistic variables suggested in the literature affect subject pronoun realisation (Definite Null Instantiation) in British English, Singapore English and Hong Kong English?

    Given the significance level \(\alpha = 0.05\), the hypotheses are:

    \[ \begin{aligned} H_0: & \quad \text{None of the predictor coefficients deviate from 0}.\\ H_1: & \quad \text{At least one predictor coefficient deviates from 0}. \end{aligned} \]

    These can be restated mathematically as:

    \[ \begin{aligned} H_0: & \quad \beta_1 = \beta_2 = \cdots = \beta_p = 0 \\ H_1: & \quad \text{At least one } \beta_i \neq 0 \text{ for } i \in \{1, 2, \ldots, p\} \end{aligned} \]

    Convert to factors and specify reference levels

    The next step involves specifying reference levels for all categorical variables. This step is very important because it will directly impact the parameter estimation procedure and, consequently, influence our interpretation of the model output.

    • The reference level of the response is usually chosen such that it corresponds to the unmarked or most frequent case. Since overt pronouns are much more common in the data, the reference level of the Reference variable will be set to Reference = overt. This way, the model coefficients will directly represent the probability of the null subject variant (i.e., the special case) given certain predictor configurations.

    • The predictor levels need to be specified as well. Among other things, we are interested in how the Asian Englishes pattern relative to British English. Therefore, we will define British English as the baseline for comparison.

    We will use the following specifications:

    Variable Factor Levels Preferred Reference level
    Register S1A, S1B S1A
    Variety GB, SING, HK GB
    Person 1, 2, 3, it, there 3
    Referentiality referential, non-referential referential
    # Store "Reference" as factor
    data_pro$Reference <- as.factor(data_pro$Reference)
    
    ## Specify reference level (the 'unmarked' case)
    data_pro$Reference <- relevel(data_pro$Reference, "overt")
    
    ## Print levels
    levels(data_pro$Reference)
    [1] "overt" "null" 

    Repeat the procedure for the remaining categorical variables.

    Code
    # Store "Register" as factor
    data_pro$Register <- as.factor(data_pro$Register)
    
    ## Specify reference level
    data_pro$Register <- relevel(data_pro$Register, "S1A")
    
    # Store "Variety" as factor
    data_pro$Variety <- as.factor(data_pro$Variety)
    
    ## Specify reference level
    data_pro$Variety <- relevel(data_pro$Variety, "GB")
    
    # Store "Person" as factor
    data_pro$Person <- as.factor(data_pro$Person)
    
    ## Specify reference level
    data_pro$Person <- relevel(data_pro$Person, "3")
    
    # Store "Referentiality" as factor
    data_pro$Referentiality <- as.factor(data_pro$Referentiality)
    
    ## Specify reference level
    data_pro$Referentiality <- relevel(data_pro$Referentiality, "referential")

    Model fitting

    # With (glm); available in base R
    # Note the additional "family" argument!
    Reference.glm <- glm(Reference ~ Register + Variety + Person + Referentiality, data = data_pro, family = "binomial")
    
    # View model statistics
    summary(Reference.glm)
    
    Call:
    glm(formula = Reference ~ Register + Variety + Person + Referentiality, 
        family = "binomial", data = data_pro)
    
    Deviance Residuals: 
        Min       1Q   Median       3Q      Max  
    -0.6257  -0.2719  -0.2512  -0.1597   3.1990  
    
    Coefficients: (1 not defined because of singularities)
                                  Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)    
    (Intercept)                    -3.4402     0.2221 -15.489  < 2e-16 ***
    RegisterS1B                     0.1102     0.1603   0.688  0.49173    
    VarietyHK                       0.9856     0.2248   4.385 1.16e-05 ***
    VarietySING                     0.9662     0.2272   4.252 2.12e-05 ***
    Person1                        -0.9155     0.1800  -5.086 3.65e-07 ***
    Person2                        -1.6676     0.2691  -6.198 5.72e-10 ***
    Personit                        0.8130     0.2960   2.747  0.00602 ** 
    Personthere                    -2.6367     1.0078  -2.616  0.00889 ** 
    Referentialitynon-referential       NA         NA      NA       NA    
    ---
    Signif. codes:  0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
    
    (Dispersion parameter for binomial family taken to be 1)
    
        Null deviance: 1498.8  on 4837  degrees of freedom
    Residual deviance: 1387.5  on 4830  degrees of freedom
    AIC: 1403.5
    
    Number of Fisher Scoring iterations: 7
    # Get Pseudo-R^2 (requires the DescTools library)
    DescTools::PseudoR2(Reference.glm, "Nagelkerke")
    Nagelkerke 
    0.08538986 
    # Likelihood ratio test
    lmtest::lrtest(Reference.glm)
    Likelihood ratio test
    
    Model 1: Reference ~ Register + Variety + Person + Referentiality
    Model 2: Reference ~ 1
      #Df  LogLik Df  Chisq Pr(>Chisq)    
    1   8 -693.75                         
    2   1 -749.42 -7 111.33  < 2.2e-16 ***
    ---
    Signif. codes:  0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1

    Further model diagnostics

    Post-hoc evaluation of the logistic regression model is just as important as it is for linear regression. However, several assumptions can be relaxed. While independence of observations, a linear relationship between predictors and response as well as uncorrelated predictors remain essential, the stipulations on the distribution and constant variance of the residuals may be disregarded.

    Multicollinearity can be inspected via the vif() function from the car package:

    Code
    vif(Reference.glm)
                      RegisterS1B                     VarietyHK 
                               NA                            NA 
                      VarietySING                       Person1 
                               NA                            NA 
                          Person2                      Personit 
                               NA                            NA 
                      Personthere Referentialitynon-referential 
                               NA                            NA 

    The fact that the variance inflation factors cannot be estimated properly is highly problematic, suggesting severe multicollinearity. In fact, checking for linear dependencies via alias() reveals Referentiality = non-referential as the culprit:

    alias(Reference.glm)
    Model :
    Reference ~ Register + Variety + Person + Referentiality
    
    Complete :
                                  (Intercept) RegisterS1B VarietyHK VarietySING
    Referentialitynon-referential 0           0           0         0          
                                  Person1 Person2 Personit Personthere
    Referentialitynon-referential 0       0       1        1          

    Refitting the model without said predictor solves the problem:

    Reference.glm2 <- glm(Reference ~ Register + Variety + Person, data = data_pro, family = "binomial")
    
    summary(Reference.glm2)
    
    Call:
    glm(formula = Reference ~ Register + Variety + Person, family = "binomial", 
        data = data_pro)
    
    Deviance Residuals: 
        Min       1Q   Median       3Q      Max  
    -0.6257  -0.2719  -0.2512  -0.1597   3.1990  
    
    Coefficients:
                Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)    
    (Intercept)  -3.4402     0.2221 -15.489  < 2e-16 ***
    RegisterS1B   0.1102     0.1603   0.688  0.49173    
    VarietyHK     0.9856     0.2248   4.385 1.16e-05 ***
    VarietySING   0.9662     0.2272   4.252 2.12e-05 ***
    Person1      -0.9155     0.1800  -5.086 3.65e-07 ***
    Person2      -1.6676     0.2691  -6.198 5.72e-10 ***
    Personit      0.8130     0.2960   2.747  0.00602 ** 
    Personthere  -2.6367     1.0078  -2.616  0.00889 ** 
    ---
    Signif. codes:  0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
    
    (Dispersion parameter for binomial family taken to be 1)
    
        Null deviance: 1498.8  on 4837  degrees of freedom
    Residual deviance: 1387.5  on 4830  degrees of freedom
    AIC: 1403.5
    
    Number of Fisher Scoring iterations: 7
    vif(Reference.glm2)
    RegisterS1B   VarietyHK VarietySING     Person1     Person2    Personit 
       1.036261    2.018748    2.023863    1.129088    1.087312    1.098571 
    Personthere 
       1.006525 

    Removing it does not affect the fit at all:

    anova(Reference.glm, Reference.glm2)
    Analysis of Deviance Table
    
    Model 1: Reference ~ Register + Variety + Person + Referentiality
    Model 2: Reference ~ Register + Variety + Person
      Resid. Df Resid. Dev Df Deviance
    1      4830     1387.5            
    2      4830     1387.5  0        0
    Stepwise variable selection

    With the function drop1(), it is possible to successively remove variables from the complex model to ascertain which ones improve the model significantly (i.e., decrease the deviance and AIC scores).

    drop1(Reference.glm, test = "Chisq")
    Single term deletions
    
    Model:
    Reference ~ Register + Variety + Person + Referentiality
                   Df Deviance    AIC    LRT  Pr(>Chi)    
    <none>              1387.5 1403.5                     
    Register        1   1388.0 1402.0  0.471    0.4926    
    Variety         2   1413.7 1425.7 26.179 2.067e-06 ***
    Person          3   1471.1 1481.1 83.607 < 2.2e-16 ***
    Referentiality  0   1387.5 1403.5  0.000              
    ---
    Signif. codes:  0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1

    The output suggests that Register and Referentiality do not render the model substantially worse upon their removal.

    Confidence intervals and odds ratios

    # Tidy the model output
    tidy_model <- tidy(Reference.glm2, conf.int = TRUE)
    
    # Remove intercept, compute odds ratios and their CIs
    (tidy_model <- tidy_model %>% 
      filter(term != "(Intercept)") %>% 
      mutate(
        odds_ratio = exp(estimate),
        odds.conf.low = exp(conf.low),
        odds.conf.high = exp(conf.high)
      )
    )
    # A tibble: 7 × 10
      term       estimate std.error statistic  p.value conf.low conf.high odds_ratio
      <chr>         <dbl>     <dbl>     <dbl>    <dbl>    <dbl>     <dbl>      <dbl>
    1 RegisterS…    0.110     0.160     0.688 4.92e- 1   -0.206     0.423     1.12  
    2 VarietyHK     0.986     0.225     4.38  1.16e- 5    0.556     1.44      2.68  
    3 VarietySI…    0.966     0.227     4.25  2.12e- 5    0.532     1.43      2.63  
    4 Person1      -0.916     0.180    -5.09  3.65e- 7   -1.27     -0.567     0.400 
    5 Person2      -1.67      0.269    -6.20  5.72e-10   -2.23     -1.17      0.189 
    6 Personit      0.813     0.296     2.75  6.02e- 3    0.202     1.37      2.25  
    7 Personthe…   -2.64      1.01     -2.62  8.89e- 3   -5.51     -1.12      0.0716
    # ℹ 2 more variables: odds.conf.low <dbl>, odds.conf.high <dbl>

    Visualisation

    • Plot model coefficients:
    Code
    # Create the coefficient plot
    ggplot(tidy_model, aes(x = estimate, y = term)) +
      geom_point() +
      geom_errorbarh(aes(xmin = conf.low, xmax = conf.high), height = 0.2) +
      geom_vline(xintercept = 0, linetype = "dashed", color = "steelblue") +
      theme_minimal() +
      labs(
        x = "Coefficient Estimate (log-odds)",
        y = "Predictor",
        title = "Coefficient Estimates with Confidence Intervals",
        caption = "*Note that the CIs of singificant predictors do not include 0."
      )

    Code
    # Plot odds ratios
    ggplot(tidy_model, aes(x = exp(estimate), y = term)) +
      geom_point() +
      geom_errorbarh(aes(xmin = odds.conf.low, xmax = odds.conf.high), height = 0.2) +
      geom_vline(xintercept = 1, linetype = "dashed", color = "steelblue") +
      theme_minimal() +
      labs(
        x = "Coefficient Estimate (odds ratios)",
        y = "Predictor",
        title = "Odds ratios with Confidence Intervals",
        caption = "*Note that the CIs of singificant predictors do not include 1."
      )

    Interpret the model

    The full logistic regression model is significantly better than a null model with no predictors (Likelihood Ratio Test: \(\chi^2 = 111.33\), \(df = 7\), \(p < 0.001\)) and has acceptable fit (Nagelkerke’s-\(R^2\) = \(0.09\)). However, heavy multicollinearity has been detected and traced back to Referentiality; removing this feature resolved the issue and did not affect model fit.

    The model coefficients of the reduced model indicate that null subjects are significantly more likely in Singapore English compared to British English (Estimate = 0.97, 95% CI [0.53, 1.43], \(p < 0.001\)). This effect is moderate with an \(OR\) of 2.63 (95% CI [1.70, 4.16]), suggesting that the odds of subject omission are approximately 2.6 times higher in the Singaporean variety.

    …

    References

    Agresti, Alan, and Maria Kateri. 2022. Foundations of Statistics for Data Scientists: With r and Python. Boca Raton: CRC Press.
    Buskin, Vladimir. 2025. “Definite Null Instantiation in English(es): A Usage-based Construction Grammar approach.” Constructions and Frames. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1075/cf.24007.bus.
    Hosmer, David W., and Stanley Lemeshow. 2008. Applied Logistic Regression. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley.
    James, Gareth, Daniela Witten, Trevor Hastie, and Robert Tibshirani. 2021. An Introduction to Statistical Learning: With Applications in r. New York: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-0716-1418-1.
    Levshina, Natalia. 2015. How to Do Linguistics with r: Data Exploration and Statistical Analysis. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
    Winter, Bodo. 2020. Statistics for Linguists: An Introduction Using r. New York; London: Routledge.
    Wood, Simon N. 2006. Generalized Additive Models: An Introduction with R. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC.
    6.1 Linear regression
    6.3 Mixed-effects regression